天美视频

Courtesy: Debbi Amaral

About the Author

Eric Stinton

Eric Stinton is a writer and teacher from Kailua. You can follow his work through his newsletter at聽.


There are good reasons to support pre-K education without resorting to unsupported claims.

If you鈥檝e spent any time working in education, you know how often data is conscripted to justify change, big and small. Which is good! Reform should be based upon data to whatever extent possible. Vibes alone can鈥檛 improve educational outcomes.

Yet in a recent report on statewide kindergarten readiness 鈥 defined by skills such as sharing, using scissors and recognizing shapes and letters 鈥 there was a noticeable absence of data about the academic benefits of early childhood education, despite a preponderance of confident assertions from various education leaders.

Kerrie Urosevich, executive director of , was not surprised by the poor overall results of this year鈥檚 kindergarten readiness assessment, because 鈥淲e don鈥檛 have equitable access to early learning programs across the islands.鈥 

Department of Education Assistant Superintendent Teri Ushijima noted that starting kindergarten with the aforementioned skills leads to 鈥渜uicker progress toward longer-term goals, like reading at grade level by third grade.鈥

Third grade is an interesting cutoff point to mention; by then, the data tells us, academic benefits gained from pre-K education tend to disappear. 

Advocates of pre-K education often point to the 1960s and the 1970s as evidence that high quality preschool results in a wide range of benefits later in life: being more likely to graduate high school and own a home, and less likely to commit crimes or experience teenage pregnancy.

驶Unsupported By Any Available Evidence驶

That鈥檚 great news, but upon further scrutiny, the studies have been criticized for their small sample sizes and significant investments that can鈥檛 be replicated at scale. 

鈥淐ontemporary preschool programs are not like these intensive small-scale demonstration programs,鈥 Dale Farran and Mark Lipsey, who conducted of pre-K programs in Tennessee. 鈥淭o assert that these same outcomes can be achieved at scale by pre-K programs that cost less and don鈥檛 look the same is unsupported by any available evidence.鈥

Even the results of those best-case scenarios seem less impressive the longer you look at them: the , for instance, that if we take a small group of largely homogenous at-risk students, put them in classes with a 1:4 ratio of teachers to students and invest more than $15,000 per student, then only one in three students will get arrested five or more times in adulthood instead of one in two.聽

And those are the best results we have, so unique that they can鈥檛 be reasonably scaled.

Lt. Gov. Sylvia Luke promoted a public-private effort to increase the number of preschool seats statewide last year. (David Croxford/Civil Beat/2023)

Larger, more recent studies 鈥 with better methodology including random assignment of students to the pre-K and control groups 鈥 show much more mixed results. The aforementioned study from Tennessee showed that students who enrolled in early childhood programs experienced significant short-term gains compared to their peers, but by the end of kindergarten the students who did not attend had already caught up.

Worse yet, by second grade, the students who did not attend a pre-K program outperformed the students who did on cognitive assessments. Teachers also rated the students who attended pre-K as 鈥渉aving poorer work skills in the classrooms, and feeling more negative about school.鈥澛

This mirrored the results from the , which showed early improvements for students who participated in the program that faded out by the end of first grade. 

Similarly, another published in Developmental Psychology in 2022 showed that students who attended pre-K programs showed initial gains entering kindergarten compared to their peers who did not attend pre-K, but by the end of kindergarten and first grade, students who did not attend pre-K had already caught up. 

By third grade, students who attended pre-K were actually performing worse than the control group in reading and math 鈥 a trend that persisted through the end of sixth grade, when the study concluded. 

The pre-K group also fared poorly on non-academic indicators. By third grade they had more school rule violations and major disciplinary offenses than the control group, and slightly worse rates of attendance聽(trends that also persisted through sixth grade).

Yes, We Do Need Preschools

For the record, I also don鈥檛 think it鈥檚 fair to say that pre-K programs necessarily lead to worse outcomes over time, so much as they simply don鈥檛 guarantee any long-term result one way or the other. From one child to the next, from one year to the next, there are innumerable factors at play that shape how a student experiences and performs in school. The causes of academic success 鈥 especially the kind that continues into adulthood 鈥 are much more complicated than simply extending educational services an additional year or two before elementary school.聽

It would seem, then, that if we are to be as data-driven as we claim to be, we would not be touting pre-K programs as solutions for academic gaps. 

But this is not a rallying cry to shutter pre-K programs. Far from it. I fully support President Joe Biden鈥檚 call for universal pre-K, as well as Hawai驶i’s $200 million investment in building, expanding and renovating pre-K facilities. I would love to see public preschool become reality.聽

As I wrote in a previous column, one of the functions of public education is providing child care, which frees up parents to go to work. This helps the individual families as well as society as a whole. Extending child care services to include even younger children provides numerous social benefits, even if the academic benefits are limited. That shouldn鈥檛 be an issue. Taking care of children and helping families are inherently virtuous pursuits. We don鈥檛 need to justify them with performance metrics.

Science fiction writer Philip K. Dick wrote, 鈥淩eality is that which, when you stop believing in it, doesn鈥檛 go away.鈥

The reality is that, according to the best available data right now, pre-K programs are a worthwhile investment, but not for the reasons for which they are often cited. Luckily, that doesn鈥檛 mean we have to abandon early childhood education efforts. We can support them while also being honest about it. 

If we are to educate students to be data-literate critical thinkers, let鈥檚 show them what that looks like.

Civil Beat’s education reporting is supported by a grant from Chamberlin Family Philanthropy.


Read this next:

Hawai鈥榠 Needs To Get More Serious About Food Security


Local reporting when you need it most

Support timely, accurate, independent journalism.

天美视频 is a nonprofit organization, and your donation helps us produce local reporting that serves all of Hawaii.

Contribute

About the Author

Eric Stinton

Eric Stinton is a writer and teacher from Kailua. You can follow his work through his newsletter at聽.


Latest Comments (0)

I can脢禄t wait for a state sponsored Pre-K Hawaiian Immersion program. Hoping the state can partner with Punanaleo Pre-school or other Hawaiian Immersion pre- schools that already have established curriculum and routines in place.

Hauulagin · 3 weeks ago

I didn芒聙聶t go to pre-K, there weren芒聙聶t any pre-K schools where I lived. But I knew my numbers, colors, animals, objects, etc., by the time I went to Kindergarten because my parents and older sister taught me. My sister took it upon herself to teach me read before I attended Kindergarten so I was way ahead. Learning and general knowledge really comes back to the family and if parents invest time in their children. Many young parents I know look at pre-K as free daycare and really don芒聙聶t care a whole lot about what their children learn there.

Big_B · 3 weeks ago

In their 1963 ground breaking book, "Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research", Don Campbell and Julian Stanley discuss quasi-experimental designs, which are the only ones that can legally be used to study preschool. In her ground breaking recent book, "The Scientist in the Crib", Alison Gropnik and her group at UC Berkeley, report on the latest developmental psychology findings with infants and young children. Both should be required reading. From Gropnik's book: "Children who learn a second language when they are very young, between three and seven years of age, perform like native speakers on various tests. If they learn after eight years old, their performance declines gradually but consistently, especially during puberty." Then there is the contribution of grit to learning in school: Psychology, 2018, volume 9. Good luck!

KKF · 3 weeks ago

Join the conversation

About IDEAS

IDEAS is the place you'll find essays, analysis and opinion on public affairs in Hawaii. We want to showcase smart ideas about the future of Hawaii, from the state's sharpest thinkers, to stretch our collective thinking about a problem or an issue. Email news@civilbeat.org to submit an idea.

Mahalo!

You're officially signed up for our daily newsletter, the Morning Beat. A confirmation email will arrive shortly.

In the meantime, we have other newsletters that you might enjoy. Check the boxes for emails you'd like to receive.

  • What's this? Be the first to hear about important news stories with these occasional emails.
  • What's this? You'll hear from us whenever Civil Beat publishes a major project or investigation.
  • What's this? Get our latest environmental news on a monthly basis, including updates on Nathan Eagle's 'Hawaii 2040' series.
  • What's this? Get occasional emails highlighting essays, analysis and opinion from IDEAS, Civil Beat's commentary section.

Inbox overcrowded? Don't worry, you can unsubscribe
or update your preferences at any time.